Brain drain

Mind the gap

The gap between income and achievement in school has been well documented over the years but new research finds physiological differences in the brains of the poorest children.

A researcher from the U-M School of Public Health and colleagues from Duke University and the University of Wisconsin demonstrated that children from low-income households had atypical structural development in several critical areas of the brain, including total gray matter, the frontal lobe, the temporal lobe and the hippocampus.

Brain drain graphic

Past research has made the connection between lower socioeconomic status and achievement, based on lower standardized test scores, grades and educational attainment. It now appears that this income-achievement gap might be explained by differences in the development of low-income children’s brains.

“We find children from low income backgrounds display structural differences in several areas of the brain—areas that have been connected with tasks thought to be critical for achievement in school,” says Nicole Hair, Robert Wood Johnson Scholar in Health Policy Research in the Department of Health Management and Policy at the School of Public Health.

“While we’ve known for a while that children from disadvantaged backgrounds underperform, understanding these mechanisms is really important.”

First, the bad news

The researchers examined the brains of 389 children and young adults, ages 4-22, over a six-year period from 2001-07. They analyzed 823 MRI scans, conducting measurements every 24 months for a total of three observations.

Household income was recorded in nine different categories, ranging from less than $5,000 to between $100,000 and $150,000. Neuroimaging and neurobehavioral testing was conducted. More than a quarter of the participants were from families with incomes 200 percent below the federal poverty level.

The areas of the brain they looked at are those that collectively impact scholastic achievement early on and occupational success later. The frontal lobe region is particularly important for the top-down control of attention, inhibition, emotion regulation and complex learning. The temporal lobe is crucial for memory and language comprehension. The hippocampus helps with processing spatial and contextual information and has been tied to long-term memory function.

Key findings from their research:

  • With regard to volume of gray matter, children of families below 1.5 times the federal poverty level (FPL) are 3-4 percentage points below developmental norms for their sex and age. This increases to 7-10 percentage points for those living below the FPL.
  • Children below 1.5 times the FPL scored 4-8 points lower on achievement tests.
  • Developmental differences in the frontal and temporal lobes may explain 15-20 percent of low income children’s academic deficits.
  • “Our work suggests that specific brain structures tied to processes critical for learning and educational functioning (e.g., sustained attention, planning and cognitive flexibility) are vulnerable to the environmental circumstances of poverty (such as stress, limited stimulation, and nutrition). If so, it would appear that children’s potential for academic success is being reduced at young ages by these circumstances,” the authors wrote in the paper published in the current issue of JAMA Pediatrics.

And now, the good news

The problem, Hair says, could be reversed.

“The brain is malleable,” she says. “We know that it responds to environmental conditions—positively and negatively—and continues to develop into young adulthood. It’s not that these children’s outcomes are predetermined. With intervention, it may be possible to alter this link.”

Data for the study was obtained from the Pediatric MRI Data Repository, supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institute of Mental Health and National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (HD02-3343, MH9-0002, and NS-9-2314, -2315, -2316, -2317, -2319 and -2320). SDP was supported by NIMH grant R01-MH061285.

Other authors include Jamie Hanson of Duke and Barbara Wolfe and Seth Pollak of Wisconsin.

Comments

  1. Meg Otto - 1972

    This information should help identify the correct interventions for these children. Interventions that don’t simply use the things they can’t do as the intervention. For example, perhaps blending the information on mindfulness and meditation (see article by Victor Katch in this same newsletter) would be important. Interventions that target the development of the brain as a whole, not just interventions that target the academic deficiency, seems like a good way to go.

    Reply

  2. James Harris - 1980, 1991

    Since much of the nation’s prison population has an educational deficit, it seems that this area of research would be of interest to criminal justice policy makers as well.

    Reply

  3. Andy Lee - 2002, 2008

    How much of the development and achievement gap are attributable to nature vs nurture? If the poor tend to be of lower intelligence and intelligence is hereditary, I would expect some of the gray matter and test score deficiency to be linked to genes rather than environment. Has the study been controlled for variables not related to environmental circumstances?

    Reply

  4. Catherine Gottfred - 170,73,79

    Isn’t it amazing that we can do this for 6 years with no (at least not reported) attempts at intervention? If a behavioral study school would be required to form IEP team ! Would love to see follow up after intensive language stimulation.

    Reply

Leave a comment: